翻訳と辞書
Words near each other
・ Müller Glacier
・ Müller Point
・ Müller Wiseman Dairies
・ Müller's Bornean gibbon
・ Müller's giant Sunda rat
・ Müller's larva
・ Müller's maneuver
・ Müller's platanna
・ Müller's sign
・ Müller-Breslau's principle
・ Müller-Lyer illusion
・ Müller-Thurgau
・ Müllerberget
・ Müllerian
・ Müllerian agenesis
Müllerian mimicry
・ Müllers Marionetten-Theater
・ Müllheim
・ Müllheim, Switzerland
・ Müllheim-Wigoltingen railway station
・ Mülligen
・ Mülln
・ Müllrose
・ Mülmecke
・ Mülmisch
・ Mülsen
・ Mülverstedt
・ Mümin Çılgın
・ Mümling
・ Mümliswil-Ramiswil


Dictionary Lists
翻訳と辞書 辞書検索 [ 開発暫定版 ]
スポンサード リンク

Müllerian mimicry : ウィキペディア英語版
Müllerian mimicry

Müllerian mimicry is a natural phenomenon in which two or more distasteful species, that may or may not be closely related and share one or more common predators, have come to mimic each other's warning signals. It is named after the German naturalist Fritz Müller, who first proposed the concept in 1878.
The phenomenon can be understood by imagining two distasteful species that do not resemble one another and are also prey to a common predator. Occasionally, individuals of the predatory third species will encounter one or the other type of noxious prey, and thereafter avoid it. Predators that avoid only one of the harmful species provide no benefit to individuals of the other species. Therefore, it would be advantageous if the appearances of the two prey species were more similar. This is because a predator that learns to avoid either species in a pair of species exhibiting Müllerian mimicry learns, in effect, to avoid both.
==Background==

Müllerian mimicry was proposed by the German zoologist and naturalist Johann Friedrich Theodor Müller (1821–1897), always known as Fritz. An early proponent of evolution, Müller offered the first explanation for resemblance between certain butterflies that had puzzled the English naturalist Henry Walter Bates, who, like Müller, spent a significant part of his life in Brazil. Müller had also seen these butterflies first hand, and collected specimens like Bates.
Understanding Müllerian mimicry is impossible without first understanding aposematism, or warning signals. Dangerous organisms with these honest signals are avoided by predators, who quickly learn after a bad experience not to pursue the same prey again. Learning is not actually necessary for animals which instinctively avoid certain prey; however, learning from experience is much more common.〔Wickler, Wolfgang (1998). "Mimicry". ''Encyclopædia Britannica'', 15th edition. Macropædia 24, 144–151. http://www.britannica.com/eb/article-11910〕 The underlying concept with predators that learn is that the warning signal makes the harmful organism easier to remember than if it remained as cryptic as possible (e.g. being still and silent, providing no scent, and being well camouflaged). Aposematism and crypsis are in this way opposing concepts, but this does not mean they are mutually exclusive. Many animals remain inconspicuous until threatened, then suddenly employ warning signals, such as startling eyespots, bright colors on their undersides or loud vocalizations. In this way, they enjoy the best of both strategies. These strategies may also be employed differentially throughout stages of development. For instance, large white butterflies are aposematic as larvae, but are Müllerian mimics once they emerge from the final stages of development as butterflies.
Many different prey of the same predator may employ separate warning colors, but this makes no sense for any party. Surely if they could all get together and agree on a common warning signal, the predator would have fewer detrimental experiences, and the prey would lose fewer individuals educating it. But no such conference needs to take place, as a prey species that just so happens to look a little like a harmful species will be safer than its conspecifics, leading to a tendency toward a single warning language. This can lead to the evolution of both Batesian and Müllerian mimicry, depending on whether the prey is harmful, as well, or just a free-rider. Multiple species can join this protective cooperative, expanding the mimicry ring.
Müller thus provided an explanation for 'Bates' paradox'; the mimicry was not a case of exploitation by one species, but rather a mutualistic arrangement.

抄文引用元・出典: フリー百科事典『 ウィキペディア(Wikipedia)
ウィキペディアで「Müllerian mimicry」の詳細全文を読む



スポンサード リンク
翻訳と辞書 : 翻訳のためのインターネットリソース

Copyright(C) kotoba.ne.jp 1997-2016. All Rights Reserved.